The Brain and Language


Professor Dave again, let’s learn about language. We just learned about brain lateralization,
or the notion that certain cognitive functions can be localized more specifically in one
hemisphere of the brain over the other. Language is one such function, as it is highly
localized in the left hemisphere. Attempts to understand why this is the case
can help us understand the evolution of cerebral lateralization in general, so let’s take
a look at how the brain processes language. The motor theory of lateralization proposes
that the left hemisphere controls fine movements, speech being one of these, but our ability
to make a variety of noises with our mouths is far from the full story when it comes to language. Unlike other animals, we are able to take
a finite set of elements, which comprise some alphabet, and use them to express any idea
imaginable, new or old, concrete or abstract. Even monkeys and whales and dolphins, which
do exhibit rudimentary forms of speech, can’t do what we do. They do not have such fine motor control over
speech so as to produce the distinct syllables that we can. But they are also not as hard-wired as we
are to be able to perceive and comprehend these sounds, something that is innate in
humans, which are able to learn their parents’ languages remarkably quickly in infancy. What are the specific areas of the brain that
are responsible for this capacity? Two critical regions are called Broca’s
area and Wernicke’s area. The first of these is responsible for the
motor functions that allow us to formulate syllables with our mouths, since damage to
this area results in impairment of this function, without affecting the ability to understand language. The second area is the one that is responsible
for the understanding of language, since damage to this region does not impair the ability
to vocalize, but results in complete nonsense being spoken. These two regions together with the basal
nuclei form a language implementation system that analyzes words that are heard and produces
our own speech in response. A surrounding set of cortical areas links
this system with other regions of the cortex that are responsible for conceptualization
of ideas, essentially enabling us to communicate our thoughts. This highlights the features of language that
are lateralized, though there are functions of the opposing hemisphere that are also relevant,
which allow us to interpret the nonverbal components of language, such as body language,
gestures, and any kind of tone or quality that would signify the emotional content of
what is heard. Much of what we have mentioned so far can
be considered part of the Wernicke-Geschwind model. This is comprised of seven components, two
of which are Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area, which we just discussed. The others are the primary visual cortex,
angular gyrus, primary auditory cortex, arcuate fasciculus, and the primary motor cortex. All of these are in the left hemisphere of
the brain, not surprisingly. Now let’s trace the brain activity that
occurs during a conversation. According to this model, when you are listening
to someone talk, their voice is converted into signals that are sent to the primary
auditory cortex, and then conducted to Wernicke’s area. This is where we can imagine that the words
are actually comprehended, as this is where the neural representation of the thought underlying
the reply is generated, which is then sent via the arcuate fasciculus over to Broca’s area. From there, information is sent to the primary
motor cortex, which controls the muscles in your mouth so that you may respond. Instead, when reading aloud, the visual information
of the written words is sent to the primary visual cortex, and this information is transmitted
to the angular gyrus, which translates the written word into the corresponding auditory
signal, and sends that to Wernicke’s area for comprehension. The rest follows the same path already outlined. We should note that this is simply a model,
and is likely somewhat of an oversimplification of brain function. There is empirical evidence to support this
model, relating to patients with brain injuries in specific areas, and the observation of
the type and severity of function loss. But the model is not flawless, it can’t
account for all of the data, and not all of its predictions have been confirmed. But it is a reasonable model that serves as
a basic understanding of how the brain perceives and produces language. There is plenty more to discuss here, and
it is up for debate the degree to which we are innately programmed to learn language,
with syntactical and grammatical rules fully formed in the brain, according to Noam Chomsky’s
theory of universal grammar. We will touch on this again in a future linguistics
course, but for now let’s now move on to some other topics.

19 thoughts on “The Brain and Language

  1. I passed my chemistry physics of bachelor of science too easily as it is smoothly explained by professor Dave 💯

  2. It is a pity that you mentioned Chomsky, who is now known much more known for his political work than his theories of language, which are now discredited. You perhaps should have mentioned Whitney's The Psychology of Language for further reading.

  3. I feel like I’m going to be watching you a ton next semester in physics. Studying to be a radiation therapist 🙂

  4. When you are bilingual or more do you think in the language that you are going to speak in? Just curious because I know maybe 20 words in Spanish, but it comes from thoughts in English.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *